top of page

How Leading Questions Shape Eyewitness Testimony

Chuan Lin


Understanding the impact of leading questions in eyewitness testimony is very important because eyewitness evidence is often used to decide whether someone is guilty or innocent. It made sense since someone who saw the crime couldn't be more reliable than someone else.  However, there's more to the story than meets the eye. According to research, memory is not a perfect record but rather a changing and unsteady thing that can change based on outside events. Some might say it's smart to ask questions. These words suggest actions that can lead to false memories without the viewer being aware of it. A harmless memory could turn into something completely different, which would seriously call justice and truth into question.  


Leading Questions

Leading questions that work are very strong, even though they seem simple. Small clues could help to change what a witness remembers. In relation to this. "Was it red?" is more interesting than "What color was the car?" More so if the witness looks confused, a small push toward "red" might be enough to replace the first memory. In her groundbreaking 1974 study, the famous scientist Elizabeth Loftus was the first to show this. People had to rate the speed of the cars in shots of a crash. Someone heard "hit," while someone else thought the question was "smashed." People who heard the word "smashed" thought it meant faster speeds and broken glass, but this did not happen. With just one line, they changed how they felt about it. It's scary to think about how easy it could be to control someone's memory—not with a lot of planning, but with just a question.  


Memories may seem important, but they are really just rough drafts that are always being improved, like a history painting. Memory, on the other hand, is restorative, which means it doesn't exactly play back events. Instead, when we remember something, we fill in the holes with what we know. From this normal process come important questions. If more information is sent, it might be able to fit in the memory. Say you ask someone, "Did the suspect have a gun?" They might remember a gun very clearly even if they've never seen one. In order to give doubt meaning, the mind fills in the blanks with thoughts, weaving fiction and truth into a story that makes sense. This is how memory works; it is not changed on design. But when the stakes are that high, like in a court case, even a small mistake could bring death.  


Impact

Changing memories could have very important real-world effects. Courts are not science labs; they are places that work to protect human life, liberty, and justice. People often think that first-hand accounts are accurate, but many people have been jailed because of them. What if the memories that were so clear when the person was testifying were brought up again by the same questions that are used in police interviews? Even innocent people have been found guilty when a witness's memory was messed up by accident. For example, a witness might first remember a vague figure, but if they were asked over and over, "Was the man in the blue shirt holding a knife?" they would remember not only a blue shirt, but also a made-up weapon that they had never seen. Some people make a lot of these small mistakes, which can lead to illogical views and false stories. These kinds of mistakes hurt people outside of court because they ruin lives and make people less trusting of the justice system.  


Nature of Problem

The good news is that asking leading questions won't help you find out as much. Even though it's basic, the technique needs to be known about and taught. One good approach is to ask open-ended questions that let witnesses describe what happened in their own words without being told what to say. "Did the suspect have a beard?" is not the only question the police can ask. "Could you outline the suspect?" This small change keeps us from becoming attached to things that might not exist. The cognitive interview is another way for witnesses to reenact the event by focusing on sense data and their own point of view. This method lowers the chance of ideas by maintaining memory accuracy. If they want to do their jobs well, police officers, lawyers, and judges who have finished their required training must use these tools. Keeping personal proof honest is both the right thing to do and the better way to do things.  


Last but not least, memory's flaws are both terrible and interesting. The way a question is worded can change even someone's memory that seems perfect. The research by Elizabeth Loftus shows how easily our memories can be changed, which can have surprising effects on our daily lives. From wrong ideas to lost events, the stakes could not be higher. You can still help us grow, though. Knowing how memories can be used to rebuild things and asking objective, open-ended questions will help lower the expected error potential. Truth is necessary for justice, and it also makes sure that memories are correct. It's clear that we all have a duty to protect witnesses from the minor effects of influence and make sure that their statements are honest and not fabricated.  


References

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 585–589.  


Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.


Wells, G. L., Memon, A., & Penrod, S. D. (2006). Eyewitness evidence: Improving its probative value. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(2), 45–75.  














Comments


Contact Us!
or email us @veritasnewspaperorg.gmail.com

Thanks for submitting! We will contact you via email - make sure to check your spam folder as our emails sometimes appear there.

veritas.pdf (1).png

© 2025 by Veritas Newspaper

bottom of page